Case Problems |
CONTENTS:
PART I: SAMPLE SOLVED PROBLEMS:
A good starting point in development
of critical thinking skills is use of authentic examples meaningful to the student.
The popular media are rich in such material --
sports physiology, reproductive health, nutrition, fad diets,
psychoactive drugs, alternative
therapies, pollution, genetic engineering, and evolution.
The following examples show how active learning can be incorporated
into the lecture theater environment to target critical thinking skills. Active learning
requires that the students themselves grapple with the case examples, such as in temporary
small groups within the lecture hall. During small group work, the instructor can
circulate among the groups and suggest directions for student discussion.
Short lectures (15-20 minutes) are excellent for inspiring students and for demonstrating
how to attack problems, but active learning is superior to a mimetic learning
environment in which students only listen, take notes, and repeat what they have been told.
Note the brevity of these introductory cases. Brief quotations help students focus on the
fundamental scientific issues without distraction and provide ample substrate for group
discussion. Students may of course progress to the
analysis of longer articles, scientific papers, advertisements, and web pages
assigned as homework or term papers.
In each case, the students must evaluate the reliability of the
claim being
promulgated. Reliable knowledge is evidence-driven. The students musk ask,
"What is the quality of evidence supporting this
claim?"
This case provides skills and practice in evaluating a report of a scientific
investigation, and introduces students to the need for
evidence-based medicine.
Exposure of students to a diversity of health care claims is an important
component of a biology education. Health care is a multi-billion dollar biology-based
industry, but with many examples of pseudoscience, deception and fraud.
Scientific reasoning is an indispensable tool for the citizenry in evaluating
health care claims.
Alternative therapies such as homeopathy, reflexology, acupressure,
and therapeutic touch are often of interest to students and their families, and
can be critically examined in a biology course on a topical basis,
e.g., iridology when studying the
biology of the eye.
Students can critique the evidence for alternative medicine
against a rigorous scientific protocol. Pathologist and consumer advocate Marcia Angell has
repeatedly stated that there
are but two kinds of medicine: that which has been adequately tested and that which
has not (Angell and Kassirer 1998).
Example:
Ask the students to form groups of three to five within the lecture room or assign groups by
lottery. Provide the following brief news report
to analyze. Tell the groups that you may call upon them
when the class reconvenes after 15 minutes of small group work.
Ask the groups to decide whether or not the claim in the report is
justified by the evidence cited in the report.
The treatment involved heating an acupuncture
point upon the smallest toe ("moxibustion"). Two hundred and sixty women with
poorly positioned
fetuses were studied. Half of the
women were randomly selected
to receive moxibustion. The other women ["controls"]
received no treatment. The investigators found that the untreated women
had significantly more breech births. The authors stated that previously no randomized controlled
trial [like this one] had ever been conducted.
Ask the students to form their decision using the following five criteria:
Reconvene the class and solicit responses from the groups for an instructor-led
discussion with the class
as a whole. Comments may be similar to the following.
The claim that moxibustion is helpful in positioning of the fetus is not
well justified. A major flaw in the evidence is the lack of proper controls.
A better test of
this claim would involve double-blinding, that is giving a sham treatment
to the control group,
thus isolating the independent variable.
Also, the claim is implausible as there is no known mechanism. It is especially
desirable to determine if other investigators
can repeatedly reproduce the results using better quality methods. Johnson (1999)
states, "If results from a study cannot be reproduced, they have no credibility ...
Individual studies rarely contain sufficient information to support a final
conclusion about the truth or value of a hypothesis." The next example
addresses this issue in more depth.
Critical thinking skills may be tested in large classes using multiple-choice exams.
[An example is shown below.]
WEIGHING CONFLICTING EVIDENCE
This case introduces skills and practice in weighing conflicting evidence
and detecting publication bias. It is a surprise to many students that a multitude of apparently
similar experiments may produce somewhat differing results, but this is a crucial point to address in
teaching critical thinking skills.
Have the students participate in a two-stage analysis:
Provide the students with practice in analyzing funnel plots.
Display or issue copies of funnel plots such as Exhibits A and B, below.
Each dot shows the outcome of a published controlled trial. Exhibit A
is a plot of the outcomes of 34 trials that investigated the effect of
hostility on the risk of developing
coronary heart disease (adapted from Petticrew et al. 1999). Exhibit B shows the results of
19 trials that
studied the effects of maternal
smoking on risk of preterm delivery (adapted from Shah and Bracken 2001).
Remind the students that reproducibility is an important criterion of reliable knowledge.
Ask the students to spend ten minutes
or so preparing an interpretation of the plots: Is there an effect of the
independent variable (hostility and smoking)?
Why are the results within each exhibit
so variable? How might we account for the shape of the plots?
When the class reconvenes, solicit suggestions
from the student groups, and assist the class in explaining the plots.
The students might suggest: (1) The shape of each plot is
consistent with
the Law of Large Numbers: greater variability among estimates from small samples.
(2) Results from the larger sample sizes in each plot
may be worthy of greater weighting in drawing a conclusion. (3) The hypothesis that
hostility increases the risk of coronary heart
disease is not well justified by the pattern in Exhibit A. (4) The weight of evidence
is consistent with the inference that
that maternal smoking may be a cause of preterm delivery.
The students may suggest: (1) The position of the outcomes of trials with
large sample sizes suggest little evidence of a treatment effect of acupuncture on stroke.
(2) The lack of symmetry is suspicious -- we would
expect additional points to the
left of the plot (in this case negative outcomes).
(3) The lack of symmetry suggests the possibility of publication
bias, a tendency for negative outcomes to not be published.
(4) Other factors also could explain the shape, such as
disproportionately poor responses of control subjects in small trials.
Recognition of the possibility of publication
bias is an important learning outcome.
JUDGING CONFIDENCE
This case introduces the concept of confidence. At first glace it appears to involve
some heavy slogging, but the numerical values used in this example are very easy to work with and the educational
benefits are immense.
Display or
issue copies of the plot below and ask the students to form groups
of three to five in the lecture hall.
The graph shows the last reported day of
cold symptoms in (a) a group of 100 people treated with an experimental
purported cold remedy designed by a biotech company (dark bars)
and (b) a group of 100 people treated with placebo (light bars).
Inform the students that they will be asked to further interpret
the data, but should use some specific tools. You may wish to suggest that they weigh any or all
of the following:
Difference in mean recovery time
Relative risk of still showing symptoms beyond six days can easily be computed (= 0.6).
The number of subjects showing symptoms beyond six days
in the treatment group was 37 and in the control group was 61. [(37/100)/(61/100) = 0.6].
Comparison of standard deviations between the groups
Confidence intervals
P value
Risk of error
Effect size
Likely the students will point out that
although there is a statistically significant difference between the groups, the two confidence
intervals are very close, which raises questions about the biological significance. Cold
symptoms are subjective and to infer symptom clearance within a time frame of a
fraction of a day is likely fraught with problems. The P value tells us nothing about the
probability that the treatment works (the biological hypothesis). The P value tells us only that the chances of obtaining
our sampled data are small given a statistical null hypothesis. This may or may not justify
inferring a clinical treatment effect. Many other aspects of the trial require scrutiny.
Make a list of potential sources of
bias and confounding variables suggested by the students.
It would be desirable to know what operational definition of a "cold" was
used by investigators. Were all subjects infected with the smae cultured virus? "Colds"
may be caused by many viruses, which may show differences among them in response to
a particular drug.
The students should note that
the numerical value of the relative
risk statistic depends upon the end point that is chosen. In this case the end point was
six days. Is there a more meaningful end point? Ultimately, the students may state that they
would like to know if the results could be consistently reproduced by other investigators.
Time permitting, you may wish to demonstrate how to derive a confidence interval on relative
risk (in this example the 95% interval is 0.46 - 0.78) and introduce the
students to graphical methods of plotting confidence intervals such as
forest plots.
Finally, you may wish to introduce some discussion of Bayesian analysis, in which the
probability of the statistical hypothesis, given the data, is calculated, P(H I D),
rather than
the probability of the data given the hypothesis, P(D I H), as above.
A Bayesian approach allows the likelihood to be modified by a prior estimate of
probability of the hypothesis. The effect of assigning a low prior probability is to
require the accumulation of more data to reach the same conclusion as a
less skeptical researcher.
Major outcomes of the discussion should focus on (a) the difficulty of
hypothesis-testing as the treatment effect moves toward zero, (b) the difference between
rejecting a statistical null hypothesis and making an inference about a biological
treatment effect, two separate operations, and (c) the reasons why the
literature on many useless alternative medicines may be
plagued by false positives.
JUDGING TESTABILITY OF A CLAIM
This case introduces the concept of testability.
A common impediment to scientific progress is failure to frame testable questions.
Pseudoscience may employ untestable metaphors as explanations
for alleged healing powers and may invoke unmeasurable variables. (Wilhelm Reich,
the psychotherapist, championed "orgone rays,"
touch therapists cite "energy fields," traditional Chinese medicine cites qi,
and traditional Indian medicine (Ayurveda) cites the tridosha.) Challenging students to dissect
such claims provides important lessons in the need for testability, parsimony, elegance, and
rigor in scientific reasoning. An example follows:
Ask the students to form groups of three to five within the lecture room or assign groups by
lottery. Provide
the following brief quotation to analyze. Tell the groups that you may
call upon them when the class reconvenes after 15 minutes of small group work. Ask the students how they would
decide if the claim were justified, and how they might investigate the claim. Encourage them
to draw upon their knowledge of biology to suggest tests of any
alternate hypotheses that might occur to them.
To investigate why some people are successful nicotine abstainers, the
students might suggest an experiment that compares long-terms abstainers to
to a control group of people who frequently resume smoking. The dependent variable in this case
is frequency (per unit time) of resuming smoking after quitting. The independent variable, Will Power, is of
course elusive. The only way to measure "Will Power" and its trappings
as portrayed in the quotation
is to measure the dependent variable,
success in quitting smoking, leaving the experiment with no independent variable! The reasoning
is circular, a common problem of pseudoscience.
Once the students identify the circularity, they can proceed only if they
frame a testable hypothesis, and this will require application of biology. The students
will have to brainstorm a list of measurable
variables that might impact on levels of nicotine addiction. They might hypothesize that
easy-quitters have less nicotinic receptors and/or nicotinic
receptors that are less sensitive to nicotine. There are many possibilities for
creative investigative designs. In terms of critical analysis, however, a major outcome
of interest to the educator is that
the students recognize the flaw in the original proposal regarding will power.
This is a milestone case for many students, as it awakens them to literature that
masquerades as science, and makes them more critical readers. Much writing on
natural history, healing, and human behavior is entangled in untestable metaphors and
therefore is divorced from the window of science. A splendid analysis
of the concept of "motivation" is provided by Chiesa (1994).
The eminent microbiologist Louis Pasteur revolutionized medical practice largely by demanding
testability and scientific rigor (Debre and Forster 1998). This was a hard-won
lesson that needs constant reinforcement in biology classes.
This case provides practice in discriminating scientific explanations from nonscientific
explanations.
Most students are aware that
some religious organizations lobby to have supernatural claims, particularly intelligent design creationism,
taught in science classes.
Media attention given to this issue is fortuitous, as it provides a dramatic substrate upon which to
confront the question "What is science?"
Rather than lecturing students on science as though it were dogma, such as the "evidence for evolution" as
is presented in many textbooks, it is preferable to actively engage the students in examining and comparing
scientific and nonscientific theories. Ask the students to
bring to class lists of criticims of evolution that they find on the Internet. These can be analyzed in small groups and
students can themselves generate a list of criteria to discriminate science from religion.
Example --
Ask the students how they would go about testing a phylogenetic hypothesis. This forces them to confront
a question that many have never before contemplated.
Have the students form groups of three to five in the lecture hall or assign groups by lottery.
Ask them to suggest an investigation to test the phylogeny
hypotheses in
the following quotation. ALSO ask them to reflect on their methodology and
brainstorm a list of characteristics of science.
The students will likely suggest that the evolutionary explanation predicts that
the fossil record would show a sequential emergence of fish, amphibians, reptiles, and birds
over a span of time geologically dated at significantly
longer than six days, whereas the creation hypothesis predicts that all life forms
would appear together throughout the fossil record, inhabiting
the
earth from the beginning of the record of life.
The students may have other suggestions, such as seeking transitional fossils,
quiescent ancestral genes, and present-day mutants.
The creationist claim, by contrast,
predicts that the fossil record would show the emergence of birds and reptiles (and all
other major life forms) simultaneously without transitional forms.
When the class reconvenes, the students' ideas can be elicited, and an instructor-led
discussion can include a review of the
the fossil record, transitional forms
( Archaeopteryx, Tiktaalik, etc.), and genetics (such as Hoxd13 pattern
found in Australian lungfish, tooth genes in birds, the Talpid specimen, etc.)
Of most interest, however, is the list of characteristics of science
contributed by the students. Any of the following
might emerge and are worthy of discussion.
The tentative language of biological science used in the quotation
("proposed", "hypothesized") contrasts
with the dogmatic language ("believe") of the scientific creationist. Creationist
Research Society literature states that "members of the Society are ...committed to full
belief" in their explanation. By contrast, scientific hypotheses are provisional and
are abandoned if they fail rigorous testing. For example, it was once thought
that plant development was influenced by "mitogenic
rays," but this idea has not withstood extensive
investigation (Langmuir 1989) and has been discarded. As such, it is important for
the student to recognize that science is simply a human behavior, not a mirror of
Truth or a mirror of Reality. The provisional nature of science illustrates that truth and
reality are human interpretations subject to revision.
As David Western has said, "Anyone who believes that science is
dispassionate and objective has never worked with scientists" (Western 1997).
Science constructs explanations based upon a language of human perceptions of the
natural environment. Scientific claims are tested by observation and measurement. In the
case of phylogeny this may involve geometric dating, fossil records, DNA typing, genome
sequencing, etc. The value of scientific hypotheses is measured by their predictive power -- whether
or not patterns of physical evidence in nature conform to those expected by the hypothesis.
Nonscientific explanations may encompass the non-observable (supernatural), may
rely upon historic writings (religious texts), or may invent untestable constructs
(psychoanalysis). In the phylogenetic problem, the creationists may rely on religious scripture
as
one form of evidence.
Competition among scientific theories is resolved through continual research. For example,
by the late 1800s most scientists accepted the Germ Theory of Disease because it had been
well tested. The popularity of the theory was not dependent upon scientists being
ordered to believe it. This contrasts with religions in
which research may be unwelcome (e.g., Galileo's testing of Copernican Theory),
and members are expected to hold certain beliefs. Membership in the Creation Research Society
implies commitment "to full belief."
Science relies on full disclosure of investigative methods and on principles of testability and
reproducibility. This promotes consensus. The vast majority of scientists worldwide
use the theory of evolution as an explanatory tool. Nonscientific disciplines are often highly
fragmented as they lack a protocol for resolving differences. The number of religions is
testimony to this trait, as are their differing positions on evolution.
Science as a cultural enterprise encourages ongoing research. Science is self-critical
and evolutionary hypotheses are continually being reshaped
and fine-tuned. Nonscientific and pseudoscientific
organizations may be wary of research and new ideas. The alternative health care movement "largely denies
the need" for research (Angell and Kassirer 1998), and organized religion has punished (Galileo)
or killed (Aikenhead, Vanini) reflective thinkers.
A few students may view religious teachings as incontrovertible, and be impervious to scientific evidence,
and it may be helpful to point out that acquisition of
new and more reliable knowledge through controlled trials was in fact exemplified in ancient texts:
[The students should point out that the claim is not justified because there was no control group. It is unknown
what percentage of people would have reported positive health changes without treatment. And most treatments
have placebo effects -- that is why blood-letting was popular for thousands of years. Recipients of biologically
ineffective treatments may claim that
"it works" because of placebo effects, but this is not evidence of a unique biological effect.
The response of a treatment group must be compared to the response of a sham-treated group (both groups and
clinicians being unaware of which group is recieving which treatment). Also in this example, it would be important
to have clear outcome measures, not just reports of generally feeling better. A discussion
of this study is available here .]
[The students should respond that he is not justified. In fact we can EXPECT false positives due to chance
even when the weight of evidence from many trials points toward NO treatment effect. And the smaller the sample
sizes used in the experiments, the greater the likelihood that
their estimated treatment effects are not representative of what would be obtained with large sample sizes.]
[The students should respond that it must be determined if touch is better than
placebo at reducing headache frequency. An investigative design must rule out coincidence.
Testimonials are not useful as evidence
because a positive results may
be coincidence or a placebo effect.
A test of the claim requires a large number of subjects (replication) who are
randomly assigned to either
touch or a sham treatment (control group) such as an inert pill.
The outcome measure is frequency of headaches.]
[This is an interesting claim, because it is quite possible that a drug may show no
significant effect on a test population yet benefit a unique subset of people. This
was seen with the drug bucindolol in treating heart failure. Therefore the question
in this case is whether the product is better
than placebo in this one person. The "target of inference" from any evidence
is this one single person, not a population. That
rules out replication. However, randomization is possible. An experiment could be designed
in which the subject was randomly given (blindly) either the herb or a placebo each time
he/she suffered a headache. The outcome measure is headache duration.]
a. Lack of clear outcome measure.
[The answer is (d), this is an example
of pseudoreplication. It is the tanks that must be replicated (Hurlbert 1984).]
[The students should realize that
they require baseline data on the risks of the two cancers.
The incidence of breast cancer is about 80 per 100,000 population. The incidence
of uterine cancer is about 15 per 100,000.]
[The students should respond that this is an example of argumentum ad verecundiam -- argument
from authority rather than from measurement of effectiveness. The number of proponents has no relation to
determining whether or not something works.]
[Science is a method of explaining
nature using hypotheses that can be tested against human observations. It does not deal with
supernatural claims which are by definition untestable.]
Angell, M and Kassirer, JP (1998) Alternative medicine: The risks of untested and
unregulated remedies. The New England Journal of Medicine, 339:839-41.
Chiesa, M (1994) Radical behaviorism: The philosophy and the science.
Author's Cooperative, Inc.
Debre P and Forster E (1998) Louis Pasteur. Johns Hopkins University Press.
Giere RN (1998) Understanding scientific reasoning. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Hurlbert, SH (1984) Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological experiments.
Ecological Monographs, 54:187-211.
Johnson, D (1999) The insignificance of statistical significance testing. Journal
of Wildlife Management, 63:763-772.
Langmuir, I (1989) Pathological Science. Physics today, October:36-48.
Nussbaum, MC (1997) Cultivating humanity. Harvard University Press.
Peters RH (1991) A Critique for Ecology. Cambridge University Press.
Petticrew, M et al. 1999. Relation between hostility
and coronary heart disease. British Medical Journal 1999;319:917 ( 2 October ).
Rorty, R (1991) Objectivity, relativism, and truth. Cambridge University Press.
Shah, N and MB Bracken. 2001.
A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies
on the association between maternal cigarette smoking and
preterm delivery. Journal of Nutrition. 2001;131:1032S-1040S.
Skalski JR and Robson DS (1992) Techniques for wildlife investigations. Academic Press.
Tang J-L, Zhan S-Y, Ernst E. 1999. Review of randomised controlled trials of traditional
Chinese medicine. British Medical Journal 319:160-161.
Western D (1997) In the dust of Kilimanjaro. Island Press.
Copyright 1999, 2005 Peter Ommundsen
To contact, click HERE
A copy of this web page is available in
Estonian, translated from English by Anna Galovich:
Key Words:
This page
illustrates how CASE PROBLEMS can
inspire students in an introductory college biology course
to develop scientific reasoning skills.
Critical thinking means seeking reliable knowledge. Many students
fail to assess the reliability of information to which they are exposed in everyday life,
let alone pursue the dissection of scientific literature. And many people are deceived
and defrauded by pseudoscience. Practice in
critical thinking prompts thoughtful examination of the role of science in society.
This is an important outcome of a biology education, and brings us closer to
addressing the Socratic dictum "The unexamined life is not worth living."
SAMPLE SOLVED PROBLEMS:
Analysis of a Biology News Report
MORE PROBLEMS
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
REFERENCES
Currently featured Biology Critical Thinking links:
ANALYSIS OF A BIOLOGY NEWS REPORT
Case Example: Reproductive Health
The 11 November 1998 issue of the Journal of the American Medical Association
reported that a Chinese remedy used on pregnant women improves the position of the fetus
for an easier birth. The treatment reduces
the risk of breech (rump-first) births.
In this case the answer is yes.
The claimed outcome pertains to the risk of breech births, and that is the outcome that was
measured -- the number of breech births.
In some cases fraudulent claims are pseudojustified by measuring irrelevant variables or
by citing
speculative assertions by "authorities" (argumentum ad verecundiam).
In
this case the outcome of treated women
was compared to that of a control group of 130 untreated women that should
have been otherwise similar. However, this control group was
not otherwise similar. Treated women knew that they were being treated
and untreated women knew that they were different -- they were not being treated.
Differing expectations between the two groups may have affected the
motility of the fetuses. For
example, the treated women may have been less worried about birthing because they knew that
they were being treated and therefore had different levels of stress hormones.
A better
investigative design would blind the women to their treatment status. The control
women would receive a sham treatment such as heating of a non-acupuncture point.
The answer is yes. One hundred and thirty pairs of women were used
in the investigation.
If only one pair of women was studied (no replication) there would be
little confidence that a difference in outcome between them was other than happenstance.
Adequate replication establishes a statistical benchmark against
which to judge the treatment,
that is the likelihood of breech births among untreated women.
The answer is yes. The women were randomly assigned to either the treatment group
or the control group. This is important because it guards against bias.
For example, if all women of slim physique were assigned to the treatment group,
unique results in that group might be credited to the treatment when in fact perhaps physique was
the cause.
The answer is no. The report stated that until now no
randomized controlled trial had been conducted. A
claim tested by only a single experiment, as in this case, is tenuous
until the results have been reproduced by a number of high quality trials conducted by
independent investigators. Results must be reproducible.
The answer is no. There is no known biological mechanism
whereby heat applied to the smallest toe could affect the position of a fetus.
If a claim is not founded in basic science, or contradicts established laws of nature,
caution is required in viewing the results. A common aphorism is that "extraordinary
claims require extraordinary evidence," or as stated by Thomas Jefferson, evidence
must be "proportional to the difficulty" of the claim.
Analysis of a Biology News Report
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
Case Example: Treatment of Stroke
Analysis of a Biology News Report
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
Case Example: Treatment of a Cold
The mean of the treated group is six days and
the mean of the control group is seven days.
These are 1.9 and 2.1,
respectively, or for purposes of this exercise could be rounded to 2.
By rounding,
a ninety-five percent confidence interval around the mean (plus or minus 0.4 days)
can be calculated in the students' heads simply as
plus or minus two standard errors. This simplified process gives the students
an intuitive feel for a confidence interval. If the
confidence intervals do not overlap, the students would be justified in rejecting
the statistical hypothesis that the difference between the groups was
random variation.
A simple explanation of a P value
can be presented regarding the data. P is the probability of observing
the data (or more extreme data) if the statistical null hypothesis is true,
that is assuming samples were drawn from the same population.
In this case the likelihood of observing such a difference
between sample means (per unit of standard error) is improbably small (P=<0.05).
Weighing risk of error requires considering whether to set the P value rejection
criterion high (P=0.05) or lower. The latter protects against a type I error (false positive).
Setting a high value protects against a type II error (false negative).
Effect size (difference between the means/mean standard deviation) is a function of sample
size. The results of fifteen pairs of random subsamples of n = 5 and fifteen pairs of
n = 60 are shown below:
Analysis of a Biology News Report
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
Case Example: Brain and Behavior
Why do some people have little difficulty quitting
smoking? The answer is Will Power. This is what psychotherapists term "intrinsic motivation,"
an underlying need for competence and self determination.
Analysis of a Biology News Report
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
SCIENCE VS RELIGION
Case Example: Evolutionary Biology
Evolutionary biologists have proposed that new kinds of living organisms arise
sequentially through time via genetic modification. For example, it is hypothesized that
amphibians evolved from fish and that
birds evolved from saurischian dinosaurs. By contrast, there are
"scientific" creationists [members of the Creation Research Society] who believe
that all basic kinds of life (birds, amphibians, reptiles, etc.) arose within a six day period.
Daniel 1:12-15 -- "Test your servants for ten days; let us be given vegetables to eat and water to drink.
Then let our appearance and the appearance of the youths who eat the king's food be observed by
you, and deal with your servants according to what you see." So he listened to them in this matter,
and tested them for ten days. At the end of ten days it was seen that they were better in appearance
and fatter in flesh than all the youths who ate the king's food.
Although several of the examples discussed above were from popular literature, students should
be made well aware that articles in peer-reviewed journals are not necessarily
scientifically credible.
For example, Skalski and Robson (1992) listed a large number of published peer-reviewed
ecological experiments that lacked randomization and/or adequate replication.
Analysis of a Biology News Report
Weighing Conflicting Evidence
Judging Confidence
Judging Testability of a Claim
Science vs Religion
b. Lack of appropriate controls.
c. Lack of randomization.
d. Lack of replication.
REFERENCES
Kriitiline mõtlemine Biology: Juhul, kui probleem.
Case-based biology
Case-based learning
Critical thinking biology